Sunday, December 29, 2019
Saturday, December 21, 2019
Factors Influencing Unemployment - 700 Words
According to economists, employment and unemployment results when the supply and demand for human resources or labour is out of balance. Supply and demand are influenced by a range of forces that are the result of the interaction of economic, structural and policy factors. Economic Factors Economic factors affect both the supply and demand sides of labour. Demand for goods and services stimulate production which, in turn, generates employment. The resulting demand for workers affects the supply side as more workers are attracted to a vibrant labour market. The market never reaches this ideal state of balance due to a number of factors. Business cycles à ¾ Agreement among economists is rare, but they do agree that market-drivenâ⬠¦show more contentâ⬠¦The matching of skills in demand with those available is a common and persistent cause of unemployment. Education and training à ¾ Companies continually complain that the literacy levels of the work force do not meet the skill needs of the economy. As Canada shifts to a more knowledge-based economy, the availability of jobs for those without high levels of education will shrink.Show MoreRelatedMacroeconomic Variables Of The Stock Market974 Words à |à 4 PagesExecutive Summary The belief of macroeconomic variables influencing the stock market has been a highly debated discussion for the past decades. There has been no clear conclusion whether or not macroeconomic variables impact the stock market or inversely. The importance of this study have been increasingly critical as not only stock agents find the critical importance but the government to implement macroeconomic policy; the solid finding of this relation will enable policy makers to efficientlyRead MoreUnemployment And Its Effects On The Economy1452 Words à |à 6 PagesUnemployment occurs when a person who is actively seeking employment but unable to find work. It is often used as measure of the health of the economy and has been one of Australiaââ¬â¢s most serious long term economic challenges in recent decades. Economic growth is an ongoing process of increase in the production capacity of goods and services over a period of time. This determines economic wealth and individual living standards . These two economic issues have been a continuous matter within AustraliaRead MoreEssay1253 Words à |à 6 Pagesmodestly economically experienced a higher electoral vote share for far right parties during the 2009-2013 time period. Furthermore, the unemployment rate and economic growth did not appear to correlate with higher levels of support for far right parties, especially in modestly economically countries. The minimal impact of these factors in relatively low unemployment countries caused an increase of support for far right parties as the narrative was that if no action was taken to move away from the statusRead MoreFactors That Affect The Value Of An Economy1262 Words à |à 6 Pagesabsolutely considered detrimental by most economists. Unemployment Rate The employment level is regularly quoted in relations to the unemployment rate; usually it is defined as the fraction of labor force presently not working (but aggressively seeking employment). Whether or not an economy is performing well in relation to the unemployment rate depends on the extent the rate is exceeding the 6-percent benchmark (Sommers, 2005). If an economy has an unemployment rate approximately 6 percent, it is consideredRead MoreResourcing Talent1247 Words à |à 5 Pagesï » ¿Resourcing Talent This report identifies and evaluates 1.1 Organisation benefits of attracting and retaining a diverse workforce 1.2 Factors that affect an organisations approach to both attracting talent 1.3 Factors that affect an organisationââ¬â¢s approach to recruitment and selection. 2.1 Benefits of recruitment methods 2.2 Benefits of selection methods 4.1The purposes of induction and its benefit individuals and organisations 4.2Induction Plan 1.1organisation benefits of attracting and retainingRead MoreA General Public Survey Of Young Adults And Older Adults794 Words à |à 4 Pagesthe nationââ¬â¢s labor market shows a decline in the unemployment rate, the percentage of young adults, ages 18-24, currently employed (54%) is at the lowest level since government data collection began in 1948. If you were working for a national survey organization doing a general public survey of young adults and older adults, what topics and questions would you design into your survey to elaborate on this finding? Survey topic as a factor influencing participation rates has become important and researchRead MoreSupply and Demand989 Words à |à 4 Pagesand demand of goods and services vary due to various factors. This paper will discuss the supply and demand of vacation to a theme park and the various factors which affect them. First, there are many reasons why people wish to go on a vacation to a theme park. The many reasons include the fun and entertainment which can be experienced at the theme parks and also the family bonds that can be formed at the theme parks. Price is definitely a factor for people when making a decision to have a vacationRead MoreEconomic Policies Of Every Country Essay964 Words à |à 4 Pages(inflation targeting), high employment etc. The policymakers have different tools to manage these issues, primarily by influencing the aggregate demand and supply, such as interest rates, requirements to the bank reserves, tax rates etc. Therefore, this is crucial to understand how these macroeconomic indicators are interconnected, such as for example output and unemployment, unemployment and inflation, and the mechanism of policy actions in each case. Thus, the aim of this essay is to explain how theRead MoreEffects Of Economic Growth On Unemployment And Inflation1150 Words à |à 5 Pages Discuss the effects of economic growth on unemployment and inflation in Australia Economic growth is an increase in the volume of goods and services that an economy produces over a period of time and is measured by the annual rate of change in real Gross Domestic Product (GDP). Economic growth is classified as one of the most important indicators of an economyââ¬â¢s performance. Australia has maintained an average of 3.3% real GDP growth since 1992. The pursuit of a stable economic growth is a majorRead MoreHow Capitalism Contributes Towards Unemployment Essay1650 Words à |à 7 PagesThis business proposal will address the idea of unemployment as a social problem in todayââ¬â¢s society. How capitalism contributes towards unemployment is an issue that has been around since the 2007 recession and continue to be an issue for the foreseeable future. A business idea to help move in the right direction and hopefully start to lower the unemployment rate in New Zealand is to start a market where people who are unemployed and willing to work can sell food and gener ate income for themselves
Friday, December 13, 2019
Cloud Free Essays
string(63) " people respond differently to the two sides of this equation\." One of the most important findings of mathematics education research carried out In Britain over the last twenty years has been that all pupils constantly ââ¬ËInventââ¬â¢ rules to explain the patterns they see around them. (Askew and William 1995) While many of these invented rules are correct, they may only apply in a limited domain. When pupils systematically use incorrect rules, or use correct rules beyond the their proper domain of application, we have a misconception. We will write a custom essay sample on Cloud or any similar topic only for you Order Now For example, many pupils learn early on that a short way to multiply by ten is to ââ¬Ëadd a zeroââ¬â¢. But what appends to this rule, and to a childââ¬â¢s understanding, when s/he is required multiply fractions and decimals by ten? Askew and William note that It seems that to teach in a way that avoid pupils creating any misconceptions is not possible, and that we have to accept that pupils will make some generalizations that are not correct and many of these misconceptions will remain hidden unless the teacher makes specific efforts to uncover them. 1995: 13) According to Malcolm Swan Frequently, a ââ¬Ëmisconceptionââ¬â¢ is not wrong thinking but is a concept in embryo or a local generalization that the pupil has made. It may in fact be a natural stage of development. (2001: 154) Although we can and should steer clear of activities and examples that might encourage them, misconceptions cannot simply be avoided (Swan 2001: 150). Therefore It Is Important to have strate gies for remedying as well as for avoiding misconceptions. This paper examines a range of significant and common mathematical mistakes made by secondary school children. Descriptions of these mistakes are followed by discussions of the nature and origin of the misconceptions that may explain them. Some strategies for avoiding and for remedying these misconceptions are then suggested. The paper ends by relating some general features of the recommended strategies to the educational theories of Jean Pigged and Level Boycotts. Misconceptions 1 and 2: Algebra mm + 6 = mm 11 The type of error highlighted in the heading for this section arose in each of the three classes to which I have taught algebraic topics. There are at least two misconceptions that could explain this kind of error: (1) that letters represent objects rather than numbers, (2) that an answer should not contain an operator symbol such encountered the error of simplifying, say, mm + 6 to mm in two different mathematical intents. These contexts were: (I) expanding brackets containing an unknown and (it) simplifying expressions by collecting like terms. Although pupils in one class were only asked to remove the brackets from expressions such as 3(m + 2), and not to simplify, some pupils attempted such simplification. The result was that many who correctly expanded 3(m + 2) to give mm + 6, went on to incorrectly simplify this to mm. Being aware that such mistakes often originate in the misconception that numbers represent objects (discussed below), I did everything I could to avoid encouraging such a belief when teaching the collection of like terms. I strongly emphasized the fact that letters represent numbers. In keeping with this, I read mm as three-lots-of- m; making it clear that mm involves multiplying 3 by m (or m by 3) and therein implying that the letter represents a number. The group coped reasonably well until they met examples such as mm + m + 6. Many pupils would have responded to this with mm rather than the correct mm + 6. Although several pupils were helped by my repetition of such mantras as ââ¬Å"adding six is different to adding six-lots-of-m,â⬠many pupils persisted in this kind of error. Misconception 1: Letters as Objects After this lesson the normal class teacher and a second observer suggested that perhaps I would have had more success with a different ââ¬Ëmore concreteââ¬â¢ approach. Both recommended that in future I use what others have called the fruit saladââ¬â¢ approach (Thirst et al. 1998). This approach involves reading AAA + b + b + AAA as 3 apples plus 2 bananas plus a banana plus 4 apples, which naturally becomes 7 apples plus 3 bananas or AAA + b, which is correct. Unfortunately this would have helped least where I needed help most, and it would also have encouraged the same mistakes! 2 As Doug French points out, if k is interpreted as kangaroos, when faced with the expression k + k + 4, the studentsââ¬â¢ ââ¬Å"instinctive thought is likely to be ââ¬Ë4 what?. And the obvious answer is 4 kangaroos, giving k altogetherâ⬠(2002: 11). This is not the only problem that can arise from thinking of letters as representing objects. If a and b represent apples and bananas, then what does ABA mean? Indeed, if letters are used in this manner and we are asked to convert the equivalence a week is seven days into a mathematical formula relating days and weeks, we could easily be led to = ad rather than d = owe (where w is the number of weeks, and d the number of days, in a given period). Given the popularity of the fruit salad approach among teachers at the school in question, I would not be surprised if such a misconception was the underlying cause of much of the confusion on the topic of expanding brackets. However, my experience with the collection of like terms led me to search for other possible explanations for such errors. Misconception 2: Operations vs.. Answers My conclusion, confirmed by the literature (Thirst et al. 1998), was that a large part of The presence of the operator symbol, +, makes the ââ¬Ëanswerââ¬â¢ appear unfinished. In short, pupils see such symbols as +, x, and * as invitations to do something, and if something is still to be done, then they ought to do it. If we have to remove the symbol by doing what it tells us to do, in this case adding, it is only natural that mm + 6 should become mm. Readers who find it difficult to understand this tendency may like to consider their own response to the following statement 3+40- 3 140 Most people respond differently to the two sides of this equation. You read "Cloud" in category "Papers" The left-hand-side looks like a question, namely ââ¬Å"What is three divided by forty? The right-hand-side, however, is not a question but simply a fraction: three fortieths. In a sense these are just two different ways of writing the same thing, but this may also be seen as revealing that in both arithmetic and algebra some expressions lead a dual existence as both process and product. M + 6 can be seen as a set of instructions for 13 calculating a numerical value, but also as mathematical object in its own right (French 2003). The resistance to accepting mm + 6 as an answer is easily understood. In ordinary arithmetic it is always possible to remove the operator signs (unless there are infinite n number), and the final answer has not been reac hed until they are all gone.. Strategies and Remedies For reasons that should already be clear, despite its obvious appeal the fruit-saladââ¬â¢ approach seems best avoided. It may also be possible to over use ââ¬Ërealisticââ¬â¢ contexts in the early stages of teaching algebra, as such contexts often lead us (and textbook authors) to use letters in ways that might invite a letters-as-objects interpretation (m means miles, h means height and the like). In the place of such ââ¬Ërealisticââ¬â¢ examples, I would suggest using number puzzles, tricks and games. Pupils find these very nagging and algebra can be a very powerful tool for solving and explaining in these contexts. In my more recent teaching of collecting like terms, I began the topic with a think of a numberââ¬â¢ trick, requiring a sequence of mental operations. When the results were collected in, nearly everyone got the same answer. Those who did not were quickly helped to realism their mistakes. Algebra was then introduced as a tool that could explain why this happened, why it is that we could all start with different numbers, but all reach the same final answer after performing the operations in question. The topic of collecting like terms fits neatly with such tricks, as the simplifications work Whatever numbers the letters representââ¬â¢, Just as in the trick everyone reaches the After working through some examples on the board, including some of the problematic form mm + m + 6, I stopped to ask the class what is funny about the answer mm + 6. The class immediately volunteered that it is funny because it contains a plus symbol. After commending this response, I briefly commented that ââ¬Å"this happens sometimes in algebraâ⬠, and set the class to work through some questions for themselves. When one student became stuck on a question of this form, all I said was ââ¬Å"Itââ¬â¢s another of those ones with a funny answerâ⬠and he immediately knew how to continue. 14 Another useful approach with students who have already made such errors is to substitute a particular value (or values) into the two expressions believed to be equivalent (French 2002: 12). This approach has the benefit of bringing about a ââ¬Å"cognitive conflictâ⬠(Swan 2001). On the one hand the pupil believes that the two expressions are equivalent, on the other they can see that they give different results when values are substituted into them. In this situation the pupil can see that something has to be wrong, and can even be invited to explain and resolve this conflict for him- or herself. Even if the pupil is unable to resolve this conflict, the awareness of it is likely to make him or her more receptive to resolutions offered by the teacher or by other students. Substitution seems especially powerful in communicating equivalence (and the lack thereof) when each of a set of values is substituted into the expressions in question. Two expressions are equivalent if, and only if, they give the same result for all values that we choose to substitute. Misconceptions 3 and 4: The Equals Sign Equals as Makes / Equals as a Logical Connective The first misconception to be discussed in this section has been present in almost every class I have taught or observed, including high and low attaining groups in all years. One instance occurred in a Y 10 lesson on finding fractions of a quantity. Students were taught a standard method for tackling such problems as ââ¬Å"Find % of 16â⬠The method is to first find h of 16 (by dividing 16 by 4) and then multiply the result by 3. In solving this problem, several pupils in the class would have written the allowing: 16 + 4 = 4 x 3 = 12. While the answer reached is correct, the written statement is not. That statement (unlike the pupil) asserts, among other things, that 16 + 4 = 4 x 3, which is equivalent to asserting that 4 = 12, which is clearly false. The same problem occurs with any two-step calculation. A similar but importantly different error is often seen in algebraic contexts, Doug French points out that ââ¬Å"it is common to see erroneous statements like: xx ââ¬â 5 = 7 = xx= 12 = x = 4â⬠(2002: 14). Again, this statement asserts both that 12 = x and that x = 4, and therefore entails the leasehold that 12 = 4. Both of these mistakes can be explained in terms of alternative interpretations of the equals sign. 15 The misconception behind the first kind of mistake is well summarized by Candida Morgan, who observes that there is a persistent use of an ââ¬Ëoperatorââ¬â¢ concept of the equals sign by students at all levels which suggests that it is fulfilling a material rather than a relational role. (1998: 81) In writing 16 * 4 = 4 x 3 = 12 pupils seem to show that they use the equals sign not to state an equivalence between two expressions but as an instruction to calculate the alee of the previous expression. So, in our example it is as though the two equals signs connect only the emboldened portions in which they are contained 16 * 4 = 4 x 3=12. In each of these cases the term following the equals sign does indeed give the value of the previous expression. Doug French explains the misconception and its likely origin as follows: Students interpret equals as an instruction to do something to determine a result rather than as a symbol that indicates the equivalence of two expressions. This arises in a natural way through the use of equals in numerical calculations. It is also encouraged by the presence of a key labeled with an equals sign on many calculators. (2002: 13-4) Misconception 4: Equals as a Logical Connective The mistake made in the algebraic context is different, and involves the equals sign playing a dual role. In some contexts it functions as we would expect, but in others it plays the role of such words as therefore, so, leads to, or entails. Using Freshââ¬â¢s example, we can easily restructure the incorrect statement into a well formed argument by replacing every other equals sign with a genuine connective. In this way, xx ââ¬â 5 = 7 = 12 = x = 4, becomes X-5=7 xx=12 so, Hence, The above quoted passage from Morgan continues: Another common role [for the equals sign] is as a logical connective between statements, for example xx+ 3 = xx- 15 = xx- 18. [This is] likely to be considered to be mathematically incorrect by a secondary school teacher-assessor; a recent handbook for mathematics teachers picks out use of the equals sign as a connective as its single ââ¬Å"example of bad practiceâ⬠to illustrate writing that does not ââ¬Ëmake sense when read aloudââ¬â¢ (1998: 81) 16 such mistakes is the practice of reading the offending statement aloud. I would suggest that when using this strategy, it might sometimes be valuable to replace the word equals with a synonym such as is-the-same-as. This strategy is intended to help the pupil see the logical implications of their claim. In the case of 16 x 12, it might help the pupil to see that their written statement implies that 16 + 4 = 12, which they know is false. Again, this has the benefit of creating a cognitive conflict, a conflict between their mental methods and the implications of the accompanying written methods. This conflict can then be used to encourage the accepted interpretation and use of the equals sign. It is also important to model the correct use of the equals sign in your board-work. If the teacher uses the sign incorrectly, this can only increase the likelihood of pupils doing the same. Other useful strategies include emphasizing the symmetry of the equals sign, for example by not reversing the final line in the following piece of working out: xx=XX-3 XX+3=XX It may also be useful to limit pupils to one equals sign per line. Misconception 5: The Addition of Fractions Add the tops, add the bottoms In this section I consider an error that I encountered in teaching the addition (and abduction) of fractions to both Y and Y. The error in question is to ââ¬Ëaddââ¬â¢ two or more fractions by simply adding the numerators and adding the denominators as follows. 2 3+2 5 4 4+3 7 With both classes, I built towards teaching the correct method by first considering examples where the fractions were already written on a common denominator, and after a lesson on equivalent fractions, moved on to consider cases such as the 17 above. Despite their success in the first two lessons, many pupils found the third lesson extremely difficult, in most cases because they struggled to find appropriate moon denominators. During the lesson, then, nearly all pupils attempted to follow the correct method, although they found it difficult to use. Later homework and tests My overall Judgments about these classes were very much in line with the conclusions of K. Hart. A very common error in the addition of fractions was to use a rule ââ¬Ëadd tops add bottomsââ¬â¢. This was more prevalent in examples where the two denominators were different. It was also interesting to note that this particular error occurred more when the question was posed in computation form than in [word] problem form. (Hart AAA: 75) Misconception 5: Add the tops, add the bottoms In older children, who have met the multiplication of fractions, this error might be explained as a ââ¬Ëlocal generalizationââ¬â¢ that involves assuming that we apply any given operation to a pair of fractions simply by applying that operation to the numerators and denominators taken separately. Unfortunately, the pupils in question had not yet met the multiplication of fractions, and so the explanation is false in this case. It may, however, be true in others. Indeed, this might explain one of Hartââ¬â¢s other observations: ââ¬Å"The ability to solve addition and subtraction computations [with reactions] declines as the child gets olderâ⬠(1981 a: 79). A more likely explanation for pupils adopting this procedure is that they conceive fractions as test-scores, at least when written in their standard notation. Indeed, a test score of 32 out of 50 is usually written in the form 32 150. AS Malcolm swan (2001 : 149) points out, when we add two test scores together we rightly adopt the rule of adding the tops and bottoms; a score of 10 120 on one test and 120 on another does give an overall score of 25/40. As with misconceptions 3 and 4, a useful strategy in this case is to encourage pupils o read the question aloud, and to read it in a way that naturally suggests the correct approach. The addition 18 should not be read as one-over-five plus three-over-five, but rather as one-fifth plus three-fifths. Fifths are objects that most pupils are comfortable adding; one of them plus three of them is obviously four of them in total. A cognitive conflict strategy is also available for this misconception. The vast majority of pupils can tell you that a half plus a half is one (or one whole). But if you get the pupils to write down the question and apply their general strategy, something interesting happens: 1+1 2+2 2. I used this strategy with some success with Y. This example might not be sufficient to shake the misconception, as it involves adding two fractions with the same denominator. In that case we might also try adding a half to three-quarters. Pupils will be able to see that the answer should be more than one, and that their method gives an answer of 5/6, which is less than one. Again, the experience of cognitive conflict will not only lead the pupil to seek a resolution of their own but will make them more receptive the resolutions proposed by others. Misconception 6: Reflection The same, but on the other side of the line In an end of term test, I recall watching one pupil as she attempted to answer the following question in Figure 1 (all figures are at the end of this document). The girlââ¬â¢s initial answer is shown in figure 2. I was pleased to see her rub this answer out, believing that she was about to replace this answer with a correct one. Unfortunately she only rubbed it out to increase the length of the rectangle she had drawn. In fact, she did this several times before she was happy that she had done it correctly. Many there pupils in the class made the same error. Pupils performed much better on the other two reflection questions, in which the mirror-lines were vertically and horizontally orientated. Informs that mistakes of this kind are common: ââ¬Å"Children may have particular difficulties when the mirror-line is not horizontal or vertical but the slope of the object is horizontal/verticalâ⬠(KÃ'ËAchaean 1981: 143). The APP (1980)) 19 studies revealed a similar tendency when the mirror-line is horizontal or vertical but the slope of the object is not. For example, many respondents answered the question n Figure 3 as shown in Figure 4. The examples given here suggest that pupils may understand reflections as ââ¬Ëcompleting a shape, by making it the same on both sides of the mirror-line. However, examples discussed by KÃ'ËAchaean reveal that the given shape does not need to meet the mirror line for this kind of mistake to occur. Reflecting shapes in vertical and horizontal lines is certainly easier than reflecting them in lines of any other orientation. Reflections are also easier when the slope of the object is oriented either parallel or perpendicular to the mirror-line. Understandably, teaching on reflection often begins with examples of this sort. In a context such as this, many of the resulting reflections can be explained by a rule that does not extend to more complex cases. The rule is this: flip the object over in such a way as to leave itââ¬â¢s slope unchanged, now translate the object in the direction of its slope until it lies on the other side of the mirror-line, and at the same distance from that line. This is much easier to do than to explain. This rule fits the case of Figures 3 and 4 exactly, and makes the answer in fugue 2 appear quite natural the slight lack of fit combined tit the obvious fact that the object (and so the image) meets the mirror-line in two places, also seems to explain the girlââ¬â¢s dithering over exactly what length to make the resulting rectangle. KÃ'ËAchaean suggests that these misconceptions may be best avoided by tying the idea of reflection to that of folding, and the essential properties of reflection teased out through investigative work. The actions and the representations are both highly indubitable so that it should be possible to develop such an approach in ways that are meaningful to most children. The transformations can be internalized in gradual steps, by focusing first on the actions themselves and then on the representation of imagined actions. In addition, the resulting drawings can be checked at each step by a return to the actions. The approach advocated is one that directs children towards discoveries from which the rules and properties of the transformations can be surmised and against which they can be tested. (KÃ'ËAchaean 1981: 157) A similarly investigate approach could be used in conjunction with a dynamic geometry package. This would allow students to investigate what properties of a ââ¬Å"reflectionâ⬠remain unchanged as the image, object and mirror-line are manipulated 0 discussed in the next section. Misconceptions and Theories of Learning Many of the above strategies and remedies fit well with the theories of learning due to Jean Pigged and Level Whisky. Due to his emphasis on personal interactions, particularly in the use of language, Level Hoosegows theory of learning is often thought of as a calculators theory. Whisky believed that the ideal learning relationship was that of a novice learning as an apprentice under the tutelage of an expert. Nevertheless, like Pigged, he would have seen much value in peer discussions that, ender the guidance of a teacher, overcome differences of opinion and in doing so achieve a shared understanding. Hoosegows theories also provide a theoretical backing for strategies involving reading aloud. What is at first read aloud is, of course, soon expected to become self-speech (also referred to as private-speech or inner-speech). According to Whisky, as the principle form of self-direction and self-regulation, such self-speech is the foundation of all higher cognitive powers (Beer 1997: 248). According to Pigged, all cognitive change can be classified as one of two types: adaptation and organization. Organization is a largely internal process involving rearranging and linking up items of previous learning to form a ââ¬Å"strongly interconnected cognitive systemâ⬠(Beer 1997: 213). More important for our purposes is adaptation, which itself comes in two varieties: assimilation and accommodation. In assimilation the learner simply fits new concepts, skills and information into his or her existing cognitive framework. However, on some occasions new items of learning cannot be fitted into the existing cognitive framework, and that framework must be changed in order to make room for them. This is accommodation. The awareness of a need for a change in oneââ¬â¢s cognitive framework is brought about by a realization that something important ââ¬Ëdoesnââ¬â¢t fit inââ¬â¢. For this reason, Malcolm Swan and others in the Diagnostic Teaching Project have seen Pigletââ¬â¢s views as providing theoretical Justification for their view that the best way to overcome a misconception is by engineering a cognitive conflict (Swan 2001). Many of the strategies suggested above fit into this category. When those strategies were described, they were described as strategies for use with individual 21 students. However, Swan and others suggest that the strategy may be as, if not more, effective when the conflict in question is between members of the same peer group, who must then come to a resolution through discussion. A [pupil] might superficially accept a [teacherââ¬â¢s] perspective without critically examining it, out of an unquestioning belief in the [teacherââ¬â¢s] authority. Pigged also asserted that clashing viewpoints ââ¬â arguments Jarring the [learner] into noticing a peerââ¬â¢s point of view ââ¬â were necessary for peer interaction to stimulate movement towards logical thought. (Beer 1997: 253) How to cite Cloud, Papers
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)